The Great Divide
circum-Pacific colonisation rather than spread from
the Old World that has populated most of the world, given rise to most of the genetic
lineages of human language, and colonised the New World. The entry point to the New
World was of course Beringia; but linguistic typology shows that the colonisers entering
through Beringia were predominantly coastal people involved in the circum-Pacific
colonisation pattern rather than inland Siberian people impelled ultimately by
spreads out of central Eurasia.’ (Italics added.) A final gloss on this picture
is that ‘the first colonisation of the New World was under way by about 35,000
years ago’. 22
On the face of it, of course, this appears to throw much that we have been
discussing so far into disarray. The LGM consensus, the genetic
evidence, Christy Turner’s dental evidence, together with the archaeological
evidence from either side of the Bering Strait, and the linguistic evidence of Greenberg
and Ruhlen, cohere in showing that early humankind reached the Bering Land Bridge
roughly 16,500–15,000 years ago, via an inland route through central and northern
Eurasia, with a second later group crossing the strait at about 8,000 years ago,
originating in South East Asia. Johanna Nichols’ linguistic evidence says early
peoples reached Beringia 35,000 years ago via the west coast of the Pacific rim,
travelling north from island South East Asia, China and into Siberia. Can these two
scenarios be reconciled?
Nichols’ linguistic evidence is not like the genetic evidence for
early entry into the Americas. As was referred to earlier, we may allow that one or two
more or less genetically distinct but isolated groups of people entered America much
earlier than the main group of migrants without seriously jeopardising the main thrust
of the overall picture. But Nichols’ linguistic evidence by definition applies to
large groups of people, not isolated pockets.
The answer to the discrepancy must surely lie in the uncertain nature of the
methodology of chronolinguistics. Many of Nichols’ colleagues, while accepting
her division of languages into four ‘families’, do not take seriously her
arguments about time depth; and she does not herself use glottochronology. We shall see
in chapter four that, archaeologically speaking, there is next to no evidence for the
presence of early peoples in the Americas beyond Alaska before 14,500 years ago but we
shall also see, in chapter two, that there is good geological, cosmological and
mythological evidence for why there would have been a second wave of
migrants who entered the New World much later than the first, at around 8,000 years ago,
after leaving island South East Asia and travelling around the Pacific rim. In other
words Johanna Nichols is right about the origin of at least some of the New World
languages, but wrong about the time depth. (Remember that it is the calculation of time
depth that is so controversial and unreliable in comparative linguistics.) The clue to
the disparity, as we shall also see, lies precisely in Nichols’ insistence that
there was an ancient split between Old World and Pacific languages. Why should that be?
What happened, deep in the past, to cause this split?
The next chapter will go a long way to explaining that split and we shall
also see that, on their way to the New World, some of the people who populated the
Americas underwent a series of unique events that produced in them some psychological or experiential characteristics that distinguished them
from those they left behind in Eurasia and which could have affected their later
development. We shall see that some of these special events did in fact take place about
8,000 years ago, which agrees well with the genetic evidence, referred to earlier,
concerning haplogroup M130, which is associated with the Na-Dene speakers who entered
the New World at precisely that time.
At one stage, it would have been difficult if not impossible to assemble
such an argument about deep history, but not any more. In addition to advances in
genetics and linguistics, we can say that, thanks to developments in geology and
cosmology, we now know far more about our remote history than ever before and, moreover,
these studies have shown surprising and consistent links with mythology.
As a result, we now know that myths are less the fanciful, woolly accounts
they
Weitere Kostenlose Bücher